When push comes to shove, what is a fight?

UC creates effective way to decide whether to lump together similar animal behaviors

Biologists often study animal sociality by collecting observations about several types of behavioral interactions. These interactions can be things like severe fights, minor fights, cooperative food sharing, or grooming each other.

But to analyze animal behavior, researchers need to make decisions about how to categorize these interactions and how to code these behaviors during data collection. Turns out, this question can be complicated.

Researchers at the University of Cincinnati dug into this tricky question while studying monk parakeets. In new research, published in the journal Current Zoology, the team asked: How do you properly categorize two seemingly similar behaviors? The study was led by UC postdoctoral researcher Annemarie van der Marel who worked with UC doctoral students Sanjay Prasher, Claire O’Connell, and Chelsea Carminito and UC Assistant Professor Elizabeth Hobson.

“Biologists have to deal with this question: are the behaviors that we categorize as unique actually perceived as unique by the animals?” Hobson said. “How do the animals classify those behaviors?”

UC biology professor Elizabeth Hobson and UC postdoctoral researcher Annemarie Van Der Marel (shown here with 2 other students) have a new study on monk parakeets. They came up with a better way to code animal behaviors while conducting animal behavior studies.

UC biologists found a scientific way to decide whether similar animal behaviors should be grouped together or separated for research studies. Pictured from left are UC doctoral student Sanjay Prasher, UC postdoctoral researcher and study lead author Annemarie van der Marel, UC Assistant Professor Elizabeth Hobson and UC doctoral student Claire O'Connell. Not pictured are study co-authors Chelsea Carminito, Alexa Phillips and Bryan Kluever. Photo/Andrew Higley/UC Creative + Brand

Biases can easily lead researchers astray in making these judgement calls. “If you look at chimpanzees, smiling is an aggressive behavior. But in humans, smiling is friendly,” said van der Marel. “The behaviors look similar to us but the nuances of how the behaviors are used are very different.”

UC biology professor Elizabeth Hobson and UC postdoctoral researcher Annemarie Van Der Marel (shown here with 2 other students) have a new study on monk parakeets. They came up with a better way to code animal behaviors while conducting animal behavior studies.

UC's Annemarie van der Marel. Photo/Andrew Higley/UC Creative + Brand

Monk parakeets are highly social parrots that live in large colonies where they often interact with other individuals. The parakeets spend a large proportion of the day fighting, especially top-ranked birds, which makes it easy to distinguish them from birds ranked lower in the dominance hierarchy, said O’Connell. The birds demonstrate aggression in many ways. They bite, of course. But monk parakeets also like to take another parrot’s perch through menace or sheer force.

UC biologists observed two types of this “king of the hill” behavior: “displacements”, where one bird lunges at another bird and can bite to force that bird away, and “crowding”, where a threatened bird moves before an aggressor is within biting range. The team coded these behaviors as distinct because they appeared to differ in the level of aggression, with displacements appearing as a more severe form of aggression with a higher potential for injury. The parakeets also used these behaviors somewhat differently, with displacements occurring much more often than the crowding.

Annemarie van der Marel looks through bincoulars with a tablet on her lap and a spotting scope mounted on a tripod in front of her.

UC postdoctoral researcher Annemarie van der Marel observes monk parakeets with binoculars in a bird blind. Photo/Provided

The question is: were these behaviors essentially used in the same ways by the birds, so that they could be treated as the same types of events in later analyses? Pooling two behaviors has major research benefits such as creating a richer dataset for future analyses. But pooling also carries a risk — by generalizing, scientists could be losing nuances of behaviors that convey important information when considered alone, Hobson said.

To get to the bottom of this question, the team turned to computational analysis. They created a new computer model that compared the real aggression patterns to ones that were randomized. This approach allowed the team to test whether treating the two behaviors as interchangeable caused any changes in the social structure. “On top of being a great lab bonding experience, it was an exciting opportunity to learn how to use simulations to help answer research questions” said Prasher.

In the UC study, their computational analysis supported pooling the two behaviors. For the UC team, these results will help them plan their future analyses as they work to understand the complex social lives of these parakeets. More broadly, van der Marel said the new framework can also help other animal-behavior researchers make informed and data-driven decisions about when to pool together behaviors and when to separate them.

Featured image at top: Monk parakeets. Photo/Michael Miller

UC biology professor Elizabeth Hobson and UC postdoctoral researcher Annemarie Van Der Marel (shown here with 2 other students) have a new study on monk parakeets. They came up with a better way to code animal behaviors while conducting animal behavior studies.

UC Assistant Professor Elizabeth Hobson, left, is studying the social behavior of monk parakeets in her biology lab. Also pictured from left are UC postdoctoral research Annemarie van der Marel and UC doctoral students Sanjay Prasher and Claire O'Connell. Photo/Andrew Higley/UC Creative + Brand

Impact Lives Here

The University of Cincinnati is leading public urban universities into a new era of innovation and impact. Our faculty, staff and students are saving lives, changing outcomes and bending the future in our city's direction. Next Lives Here.

Stay up on all UC's COVID-19 stories, read more #UCtheGood content, or take a UC virtual visit and begin picturing yourself at an institution that inspires incredible stories. 

Related Stories

1

From literature to AI: UC grad shares career path to success

April 23, 2024

Before Katie Trauth Taylor worked with international organizations like NASA, Boeing and Hershey, and before receiving accolades for her work in the generative AI space, she was in a much different industry – English and literature. Taylor earned her master's in English and Comparative Literature in UC’s College of Arts and Sciences. She completed her educational journey at Purdue University with a doctorate degree in rhetoric and composition. After working as a graduate assistant at Purdue and UC, she became a research professor at Miami University. It might seem from there that her career was set—perhaps a tenured professorship or a university administrative position. That might have been her path, but Taylor had her eyes set on different goals. So how did Taylor transition from literature and composition to tech entrepreneurship? She enjoys sharing that part of her story.

Debug Query for this